Page 1 of 8

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal – Vol. 11, No. 9

Publication Date: September 25, 2024

DOI:10.14738/assrj.119.17608.

Mohamed, A. M. T. (2024). Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 11(9).

234-241.

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development

Aya Mohamed Taha Mohamed

School of foundation and General Education,

Canadian College of Kuwait-AlGonquin, Kuwait

ABSTRACT

This paper explores the influence of bilingualism on cognitive development,

focusing on its effects on executive functions, language processing, and auditory

processing. The study examines different classifications of bilingualism and their

implications for cognitive performance. Findings suggest that bilingual individuals

often exhibit enhanced executive functions, such as improved attention, problem- solving, and multitasking abilities. Additionally, bilingualism may contribute to

delayed onset of age-related cognitive decline. However, some challenges, including

language interference and processing delays, are noted. The paper concludes by

highlighting the cognitive advantages and potential limitations of bilingualism,

providing insights into its role in shaping cognitive processes across the lifespan.

Keywords: Bilingualism, Cognitive Development, Executive Functions, Language

Processing, Auditory Processing, Cognitive Aging.

INTRODUCTION

Today, the majority of people worldwide speak more than one language. This not only helps

with communication across cultures but also enhances cognitive skills. Studies suggest that

bilingual individuals often excel in maintaining focus and switching between tasks due to their

proficiency in managing multiple languages simultaneously. Additionally, bilingualism benefits

individuals across their lifespan: from infants as young as seven months, who can more easily

adapt to new environments, to older adults, who may experience a slower rate of cognitive

decline. (Bialystok, 2009)

Language plays a crucial role in our lives, as it is a tool for expressing thoughts and emotions. It

is an instrumental tool for connecting with others and exchanging knowledge. Speaking more

than one language is necessary for a large segment of the world's population to get by in daily

life. A 2006 European Commission survey revealed that 56 per cent of participants could

communicate in a language different from their mother tongue. In some countries,

multilingualism is even more prevalent; for example, 99 per cent of people in Luxembourg and

95 per cent in Latvia are proficient in multiple languages. (European Commission Special

Eurobarometer, 2006) Despite the common perception of the United States as a predominantly

monolingual country, data from 2007 shows that one-fifth of individuals aged five and older

speak a language other than English at home. This represents a significant rise of 140 per cent

since 1980. (United States Census Bureau, 2012) Many Americans frequently use languages

other than English, not only at home but also at work and in school. This multilingual trend isn't

limited to the United States and Europe. The Associated Press has reported that about two- thirds of children globally are raised speaking more than one language. Associated Press, 2001)

In recent years, advancements in technology have enabled scientists to explore more

Page 2 of 8

235

Mohamed, A. M. T. (2024). Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 11(9). 234-241.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.119.17608

thoroughly how bilingualism affects and modifies cognitive and neurological functions in the

brain.

Bilingualism

The concept of bilingualism might seem straightforward in the beginning. According to

Webster's Dictionary, being bilingual means using two languages with fluency and control,

which is typical of a native speaker, and bilingualism refers to the regular oral use of two

languages. Commonly, people equate being bilingual with speaking two languages flawlessly.

This perspective agrees with Leonard Bloomfield's definition of bilingualism in 1935, decribing

it as having native-like proficiency in two languages. John Macnamara proposed a broader

definition of "perfect bilinguals" in 1967. He argued that everyone who exhibit a basic degree

of proficiency in any one of the four language skills—speaking, listening, reading, or writing—

in a language other than their mother tongue can be classified as bilingual. (Harmer & Blanc

2000)

Classification of Bilingualism

Hull and Vaid (2006) proposed a multiple-criteria classification system for bilingualism. They

define "Bilinguals" as individuals who are proficient in at least two languages, with the ability

to read, write, and speak in each. Second come "Early Bilinguals" who acquire two languages

before becoming six years old, treating both languages as their first language or L1. On the other

hand, people who started learning a second language after turning six are known as "Late

Bilinguals". "Proficient Bilinguals," who maintain strong linguistic ability and score above 85%

on standardized language proficiency tests, are also included in this classification Finally,

individuals who score below the proficiency criterion on these tests, showing less competence

in their languages, are referred to as "Non-proficient Bilinguals".

Other researchers like Paap and Sawi (2015) provided another classification. They divided

bilingualism into three types: harmonized bilingualism, double bilingualism, and delayed

bilingualism. Both harmonized and double bilingualism are forms of early bilingualism that

develop during childhood. In contrast, delayed bilingualism occurs when someone learns a

second language after the age of 12.

Harmonized bilingualism occurs when a child is raised with two simultaneous language

systems. This is common in households where each parent communicates with the child in their

own native language, enabling the child to develop two separate linguistic systems they can

navigate with ease. Another scenario is when a child who has already learned their native

language is adopted by parents who speak another language, creating a different linguistic

environment for the child.

Delayed bilingualism, the third type, involves learning a new language after childhood.

Although it's generally more challenging due to the closure of the critical period for language

learning, sufficient exposure and practice can still allow an individual to reach a high level of

proficiency later in life.

The last type is called delayed bilingualism, which refers to learning the language after the

childhood phase is passed, which makes it very challenging to miss the critical period. However,

Page 3 of 8

236

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 11, Issue 9, September-2024

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

with abundant practice and exposure to the language, the person can still reach a great deal of

proficiency in his life at later stages.

Cognitive Consequences of Bilingualism

Earlier views often regarded bilingualism as a potential cause of developmental issues or delays

in children. However, recent research across various fields, such as education,

psycholinguistics, psychology, speech and hearing sciences, and neuroscience, presents a more

positive perspective. Studies by researchers like Ricciardelli (1992) and Torrance et al. (1970)

indicate that bilingualism can have beneficial effects. It has been found that bilingual children's

cognitive systems show remarkable variances from those of monolinguals. Studies have also

revealed that exposure to two languages from an early age can impact critical neurological and

cognitive development, influencing how these systems process and store information.

(Yoshida, 2008)

Language Co-activation

Studies have shown that bilingual people have both of their languages switched on, even when

they're only speaking one. When we hear a word, it doesn't hit our ears all at once; instead, it

unfolds sound by sound. As soon as we start to hear a word, our brain gets busy guessing what

it might be, activating similar words we know. For example, if you hear "can," your brain might

also think of "candy" or "candle" pretty early on. For those who are bilingual, this guessing game

pulls in words from both languages, not just the one they're currently using.

Some of the strongest evidence that both languages are active in the minds of bilinguals comes

from watching where their eyes go. We tend to look at things that we are thinking about, talking

about, or hearing. For instance, if a bilingual person who speaks both Russian and English is

asked to pick up a "marker" from a group of items, they might find themselves looking at a

stamp. This happens because the Russian word for "stamp," "marka," sounds similar to the

English word "marker.”

This phenomenon shows that when bilinguals hear something, their brains might link it to

similar-sounding words from either of their languages. Additionally, this activation of both

languages happens automatically, meaning that bilinguals often think of words from both

languages, even if the words don't sound alike. (Marian & Spivey 2003)

Executive Functions

Research consistently highlights the potential advantages of bilingualism on executive

functions, which include self-control and the ability to manage conflicting information.

Executive functions are critical situations where individuals must repress their habitual or

spontaneous reactions, such as remaining seated despite wanting to stand up, resisting the

temptation to take candy when told not to, or choosing a new approach for doing a certain task

instead of the familiar one. (Beaver & Wright, 2007)

Most research on how bilingualism affects executive functions and other skills has focused on

children. Kids growing up in multilingual environments often have to pay attention to the

abstract nature of language. Because they learn multiple names for the same thing, they

understand the differences between words and meanings and realize that object names are

somewhat arbitrary. (Ibrahim et all, 2013)

Page 4 of 8

237

Mohamed, A. M. T. (2024). Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 11(9). 234-241.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.119.17608

Moreover, Bilingual children often show improved executive control, a feature that suggests

bilingualism may enhance cognitive functions associated with self-control (Yoshida, 2008). The

constant need to switch between languages and suppress words from one language while

speaking another may act as a behaviour that strengthens the neural pathways responsible for

self-control.

However, after reviewing the existing literature, one can figure out mixed findings regarding

the extent of the abovementioned benefits. While some studies emphasize that bilinguals

outperform monolinguals in tasks requiring conflict resolution, distraction management, and

multitasking, other research findings revealed no significant distinctions between both groups.

There are even situations where monolinguals showed better performance (Paap et al., 2014).

Due to this discrepancy, researchers have suggested that the benefits of bilingualism might be

dependent on certain factors, including the specific types of bilingual experiences. It might also

be limited to particular components of Executive Functions.

Inhibitory and Attention Control

Bilingual people often perform better in tasks that require administering conflicting

information. Take, for example, the Stroop Task, where participants need to name the colour of

a written word. What makes this task challenging is the fact the printed word contradicts the

colour of it. (such as the word "red" written in blue). The cognitive system is forced by this

mismatch to ignore the unimportant word and focus on the colour. In general, bilinguals are

better at this ability as inhibitory control- than monolinguals. Furthermore, bilinguals are

excellent multitaskers. For example, they may quickly move from organizing objects by colour

to organizing them by shape. They are able to handle these switches more quickly than

monolinguals, showing greater cognitive flexibility in adapting strategies spontaneously.

(Marian& Shook, 2021). Additionally, research has proven that bilingual individuals often have

superior attention control compared to monolinguals. This advantage is resulting from the

bilinguals' need to continuously manage and choose between two languages in daily

conversations. Researchers suggest that the ability of bilinguals to keep and selectively

concentrate on different languages mentally may enhance their overall control of attention.

This skill is beneficial for bilingual individuals as it helps them avoid interference from one

language when they are using the other. (Ali, 2023)

Auditory Processing in Bilinguals

The benefits of being bilingual reach into deeper brain areas commonly associated with

processing sensory information. When listening to a basic speech sound like "da" without any

background noise, both monolingual and bilingual teenagers show similar brain stem reactions.

However, in noisy environments, bilinguals display a stronger neural response, showing they

encode the sound more effectively, particularly its fundamental frequency, which influences

pitch perception. This means that bilinguals have increased activity in their brain stem when

hearing sounds, suggesting they're better at focusing on important auditory information in

noisy settings. (Krizman et al, 2012)

Learning Advantages

Being bilingual can lead to practical benefits. The enhanced cognitive and sensory skills

developed from managing two languages may allow bilinguals to process information from

their surroundings more effectively, improving their learning capabilities. This increased

Page 5 of 8

238

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 11, Issue 9, September-2024

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

attention to detail might also explain why bilingual people generally learn a third language

more easily than monolinguals learn a second one. (Kaushanskaya & Marian, 2009) The

language-learning advantage among bilinguals could be due to their ability to focus on new

language details while filtering out interference from the languages they already speak.

(Bartolotti & Marian, 2012). This skill enables bilinguals to retrieve newly learned words more

effortlessly, resulting in greater vocabulary expansion compared to monolinguals, who might

not be as good at filtering out distracting information.

Moreover, the advantages of bilingualism seem to manifest early on. Studies have found that

bilingualism can positively impact attention and conflict management skills in infants as young

as seven months. For instance, in one experiment, babies from both monolingual and bilingual

households were conditioned to expect a puppet to appear on one side of a screen when a

tinkling sound was played. Midway through the experiment, the puppet started appearing on

the opposite side.

To receive a reward, the infants had to adapt to this new rule; only the bilingual infants

managed to successfully make this adjustment. (Kovacs & Mehler, 2009) This indicates that

even in very young children, being exposed to multiple languages can provide benefits that

extend beyond linguistic skills.

Bilingualism and Age-related Cognitive Symptoms

So many studies have revealed that being bilingual has advantages for the brain and cognition

throughout the ageing phase. One significant benefit of bilingualism that helps delay the

inevitable deterioration of cognitive function is maintaining what is referred to as "cognitive

reserve". Craick et al., 2010, cited in (Marian Shook, 2021). Cognitive reserve is the ability to

effectively use brain networks to maintain or enhance brain function as we age.

Being bilingual may assist in maintaining this reserve by attracting different brain networks to

replace the ones that are lost as we age and by keeping cognitive mechanisms sharp. Compared

to older monolingual individuals, older bilinguals have better memory and executive control,

which can have positive effects on their actual health. ( Schroeder & Marian, 2012, cited in

Marian Shook, 2021).

Research also confirms that age-related cognitive decline and dementia can be prevented by

having cognitive reverse, which aids in postponing the onset of dementia by around 4-5 years,

according to recent epidemiological studies. (Perani et al., 2016)

Negative Consequences

One primary consistent area where bilingual children tend to lag behind is in receptive

vocabulary. Studies have repeatedly shown that bilingual children usually score lower than

their monolingual peers in understanding vocabulary in each of their languages.

This finding has been observed in numerous studies comparing monolingual and bilingual

children, particularly during the preschool years and sometimes in the early years of schooling.

(Bialystok, 2001) The exact same result has been proven by other researchers who stated that

bilingual undividulas have less number of vocabulary words in their both languages compared

to minilinguals.

Page 6 of 8

239

Mohamed, A. M. T. (2024). Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 11(9). 234-241.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.119.17608

To assert the abovementioned results, researchers collected the scores from the standardized

Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test for 971 children aged between 5 and 9 years; almost half of

them were bilingual. These children had taken part in several studies conducted in the

researchers' lab over many years. The findings revealed that the average standard score for

monolingual children was 105, while bilingual children had an average score of 95. (Bialystok,

2009)

The reason why bilinguals have difficulties with lexical access remains unclear. One perspective

attempting to justify this issue attributes it to the fact that bilinguals utilize both of their

languages less frequently than monolinguals, which results in weaker links between the

necessary connections needed for quick and smooth delivery of the speech. (Michael & Gollan,

2005).

Factors Influencing the Impact of Bilingualism

Many studies are looking at aspects of being bilingual, including language usage, language

competency, and the age at which one first learned the second language—all of which may be

related to better executive functioning. While some studies find that early versus late

acquisition of a second language is a determinant of improved executive control, others suggest

that usage and proficiency of a second language—rather than the age of acquisition per se—

contributes to improved executive control (e.g., inhibition, switching, and monitoring processes

). Higher usage and competence levels have been linked to more frequent language switching,

which has been linked to better executive control and better practice effects. (Secer, 2016)

CONCLUSION

To conclude, the whole world is heading to either be bilingual or multilingual, which leaves us

with no option but to be as competitive as the world we are living in. Being bilingual not only

helps people live a better life or have a better or a high-ranking job but also affects them as

individuals; that is, bilingualism helps develop their cognitive skills. This paper aims to discuss

the main cognitive advantages of bilingualism. These benefits start showing early in a

bilingual's childhood and extend to older ages.

Most studies drew a positive relationship between bilingualism and having better executive

functions, such as inhibitory control. This results from the ability of bilinguals to suppress the

vocabulary of a specific language while speaking the other. Bilinguals also are proven to have

more focus and attention control, and the Stroop task has demonstrated this advantage they

acquire. An interesting finding was related to ageing, as bilingualism served as an anti-ageing

defence mechanism against the natural cognitive symptoms that co-exist in old people, such as

Alzheimer's or Dementia. People who are bilingual showed symptoms at later stages than their

monolingual counterparts.

However, some challenges mentioned in the literature were encountered by bilingual

individuals. Among these challenges was the difficulty of bilingual individuals in understanding

lexical items in both of their languages and one view explained the reason for that as a result of

using both languages less frequently than monolinguals. In a nutshell, the cognitive benefits of

bilingualism outweigh its drawbacks, and future research in the field might unveil more

cognitive and linguistic advantages of bilingualism.

Page 7 of 8

240

Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal (ASSRJ) Vol. 11, Issue 9, September-2024

Services for Science and Education – United Kingdom

References

[1] Ali, A. (2023). Effect of bilingualism on cognitive development in children. Alustath Journal for Human and

Social Sciences, 62(4). https://doi.org/10.36473/ujhss.v62i4.2278

[2] Associated Press. (2001). Some facts about the world's 6,800 tongues. Retrieved from

http://articles.cnn.com/2001-06-19/us/language.glance_1_languagesorigin-tongues?_s=PM:US

[3] Bartolotti, J., & Marian, V. (2012). Language learning and control in monolinguals and bilinguals. Cognitive

Science, 36, 1129–1147.

[4] -Beaver, M. K., & Wright, P. J. (2007). Self-Control as an Executive Function. Criminal Justice and Behavior,

34.

[5] Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. New York: Cambridge

University Press.

[6] Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and

Cognition, 12(1), 3-11.

[7] Craik, F. I., Bialystok, E., & Freedman, M. (2010). Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a

form of cognitive reserve. Neurology, 75(19), 1726–1729.

[8] European Commission Special Eurobarometer. (2006). Europeans and their languages. Retrieved from

http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_243_en.pdf

[9] Harmers, J. F., & Blanc, M. H. A. (2000). Bilinguality and Bilingualism (2nd edition). Cambridge University

Press.

[10] Hull, R., & Vaid, J. (2006). Laterality and Language experience. Laterality, 11, 436-464.

[11] Ibrahim, R., Shoshani, R., Prior, A., & Share, D. (2013). Bilingualism and measures of spontaneous and

reactive cognitive flexibility. Psychology, 4(7A), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2013.47A001

[12] Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009). The bilingual advantage in novel word learning. Psychonomic

Bulletin and Review, 16(4), 705–710.

[13] Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(16), 6556–6560.

[14] Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in

bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

of the United States of America, 109(20), 7877–7881.

[15] Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied

Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 173–193.

[16] Mariana, V., Shook, A. (2012). The cognitive benefits of being bilingual.

[17] Michael, E. B. & Gollan, T. H. (2005). Being and becoming bilingual: Individual differences and

consequences for language production. In Kroll & de Groot (eds.), pp. 389–407.

[18] Perani, D., Farsad, M., Ballarini, T., Lubiane, F., Malpetti, M., Fracchetti, A., Magnani, G., March, A., &

Abutalebi, J. (2016). The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer's

dementia. Faculty of Psychology, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University.

Page 8 of 8

241

Mohamed, A. M. T. (2024). Bilingualism’s Influence on Cognitive Development. Advances in Social Sciences Research Journal, 11(9). 234-241.

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.14738/assrj.119.17608

[19] Seçer, I. (2016). Skills of cognitive flexibility in monolingual and bilingual younger adults. The Journal of

General Psychology, 143(3), 172-184. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2016.1200530

[20] Torres, V. L., Rosselli, M., Loewenstein, D. A., Lang, M., Vélez-Uribe, I., Arruda, F., Conniff, J., Curiel, R. E.,

Greig, M. T., Barker, W. W., Rodriguez, M. J., Adjouadi, M., Vaillancourt, D. E., Bauer, R., & Duara, R. (2021).

The Contribution of Bilingualism to Cognitive Functioning and Regional Brain Volume in Normal and

Abnormal Aging. Cambridge University Press

[21] United States Census Bureau. American Community Survey. Retrieved from

http://www.census.gov/acs/www/

[22] Yoshida, H. (2008). The cognitive consequences of early bilingualism. University of Houston.